Psychology
Permanent URI for this collection
Browse
Browsing Psychology by Author "Zeigler-Hill, Virgil"
Now showing 1 - 3 of 3
Results Per Page
Sort Options
Item Changes in Vaginal pH and Lubrication in Response to Sexually Coercive Stimuli(2024-01-01) Vance, Gavin Scott; Shackelford, Todd K.; Vonk, Jennifer; Zeigler-Hill, VirgilWomen’s genital response to sexual stimuli is cue non-specific, such that women display similar levels of genital arousal in response to preferred and non-preferred sexual stimuli, which is discordant with their subjective reports of sexual arousal. One explanation for this phenomenon is the preparation hypothesis, which proposes that women’s genital response to sexual stimuli evolved to be cue non-specific to reduce injury to the vaginal walls during both consensual and non-consensual intercourse (i.e., sexual coercion, forced copulation, or rape). Consistent with the preparation hypothesis, women display genital arousal (including vasocongestion and vaginal lubrication) in response to audio stimuli depicting rape. However, over human evolutionary history, rape imposed special and severe adaptive costs on women, notably loss of control over mate choice and, potentially, rearing without co-parental support a child whose sire is of unknown genetic quality. The spermicidal hypothesis of vaginal acidity proposes that women’s cue non-specific genital responsivity evolved to prevent fertilization by sexually coercive men of unknown genetic quality. Women’s vaginal pH, which changes over the ovulatory cycle, and in response to sexual stimulation, may afford a mechanism by which women prevent fertilization by rape. The current research employed a within-subjects design to compare changes in vaginal lubrication and pH in response to different audio stimuli to investigate whether women have evolved a physiological mechanism (i.e., production of more acidic vaginal pH) to avoid fertilization by rape. The main goal of the present research was to determine the effect of coercive versus consensual sexual stimuli on women’s genital arousal. Specifically, I predicted that women would produce more acidic (i.e. lower pH) vaginal lubricant in response to sexually coercive audio stimuli than to neutral, sexually consensual, and violent non-sexual audio stimuli. An additional goal of this research was to replicate and extend previous research by testing the effect of different audio stimuli on vaginal lubrication. Therefore, I also predicted that women would produce more vaginal lubrication in response to a sexually coercive audio stimulus than to neutral and violent non-sexual audio stimuli, but less lubrication in response to a sexually consensual audio stimulus. Results largely failed to support the predictions of the present study; however, several significant interactions emerged from the repeated-measures ANCOVA. Namely, the effect of experimental condition on amount of vaginal lubrication, and changes in vaginal pH depended on women’s fertility status. Discussion offers potential explanations for these interactions, addresses the many limitations faced by the present study (e.g., potentially inaccurate assessments of pH and fertility), and outlines a path forward for future research regarding changes in vaginal pH.Item Precarious Manhood And Threat-Motivated Gun-Related Attitudes And Behavioral Intentions Among Men In The United States(2022-11-03) Ray, Travis; Parkhill, Michele; Zeigler-Hill, Virgil; McDonald, MelissaThe precarious manhood thesis posits that men are motivated to maintain attributes associated with societally accepted forms of masculinity. As a result, when men feel their manhood is threatened, they tend to respond with exaggerated displays of masculinity. Prior research indicates that guns are closely intertwined with masculinity and thus may be a tool through which men can demonstrate their manhood when feeling threatened. To empirically test this idea, the current research conducted two experimental studies examining the causal influence of masculinity threats on gun-related attitudes (Study 1) and behaviors (Study 2). It was hypothesized that men exposed to a masculinity threat would report more gun-supportive attitudes and have a greater likelihood of engaging in gun-related behaviors relative to men exposed to a gender affirmation— especially in a public context. Adult men residing in the United States (N = 381) completed assessments of demographics and adherence to masculine gender norms prior to their randomization into the masculinity threat and public display conditions. Following the manipulations, Study 1 participants (n = 184) completed measures of gun- related attitudes, while Study 2 participants (n = 197) also completed assessments of gun-related behaviors. Results generally did not support a causal association between the masculinity threat manipulation and gun-related constructs, resulting in retention of the null hypotheses. However, exploratory analyses revealed significant associations between adherence to masculine gender norms and demographic variables with gun-related outcomes. Together, these results suggest that masculinity threats do not have a causal influence on gun-related variables. Rather, gun-related attitudes and behaviors are partially explained by social, developmental, and cultural factors—including adherence to masculine gender norms.Item Tried and True: The Influence of Perceived Loyalty on Friendship Perceptions and Functioning(2023-01-01) Sauls, Destaney; Zeigler-Hill, Virgil; Shackelford, Todd; Vonk, JenniferFriendships provide social support and often serve to enhance psychological well-being. As such, it is important to understand how these friendships function. One feature that may contribute to friendship functioning is loyalty; however, the role of loyalty in friendships has not been sufficiently studied. The current work is intended to examine the potential relationships between the perceptions of two forms of loyalty and different aspects of friendship functioning. Passive loyalty refers to behaviors intended to be constructive in a relationship, but that may be passive in nature, such as letting a conflict go without holding a grudge. In contrast, active loyalty refers to behaviors that are more overt, such as defending a friend against the criticism of others. Participants were undergraduates drawn from the subject pools of two mid-sized Midwestern universities. Study 1 (n = 445) found that participants viewed their current friendships more positively than past friendships and that active loyalty was especially important in predicting the perceived closeness of the friendship. Study 2 (n = 252) found that active loyalty predicted perceptions of closeness for both the individual and their friend. Study 3 (n = 474) found that perceptions of active loyalty were associated with different aspects of friendship maintenance (i.e., satisfaction with the friendship, investment in the friendship, and perceived quality of alternatives to the friendship) and that active loyalty had indirect associations with friendship commitment through these features. Although hypotheses for all three studies concerned both passive and active loyalty, passive loyalty was somewhat overshadowed by active loyalty. That is, the associations that emerged for passive loyalty often were obscured when active loyalty was included in the same analysis. However, it is important to note that the associations concerning passive loyalty often resembled the associations for active loyalty but were typically smaller in magnitude. Understanding the different potential implications of passive and active loyalty may allow for additional insights regarding how to cultivate and maintain meaningful friendships, ultimately enhancing the well-being and social connectedness of individuals